Content deleted Content added
m →Views on ambivalent prejudice: rm line break, simplify heading |
|||
(23 intermediate revisions by 18 users not shown) | |||
Line 1:
{{Notability|date=November 2018}}
'''Ambivalent prejudice''' is a [[Social psychology|social psychological]] theory that states that, when people become aware that they have conflicting beliefs about an [[
Bernard Whitley and Mary Kite<ref name="KiteMary">{{cite book |last1=Kite |first1=Mary |last2=Whitley |first2=Bernard |title=The Psychology of Prejudice and Discrimination |location=Wadsworth |publisher=Cengage Learning |year=2010}}{{page needed|date=November 2013}}</ref> contend that this dissonance motivates people to alter their thoughts in an attempt to reduce their discomfort. Depending on the situation or context that has primed them, people will give priority to either the positive beliefs or the negative beliefs, leading to a corresponding behavioral shift known as ''response amplification''.
==Theoretical framework==
According to [[Susan Fiske]], there are two underlying characteristics of [[Social stigma|stigmatized]] groups around the world:<ref name = "Fiske2012">{{cite journal |last= Fiske|first= Susan T.|date= January 2012|title= Managing Ambivalent Prejudices: Smart-but-Cold and Warm-but-Dumb Stereotypes
==Views==
According to Whitley and Kite, ambivalent prejudice comes from one person having both good and bad thoughts about an outgroup.<ref name="KiteMary" /> The example in their book ''The Psychology of Prejudice and Discrimination'' talks about [[race (human classification)|race]] and how some people often have ambivalent attitudes towards people of other races. This means that their behavior is also ambivalent: "sometimes it is positive, sometimes negative
Irwin Katz said that ambivalent prejudice
Irwin Katz and Glen Hass (1988) believed that contradicting American values are to blame for ambivalent prejudice.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.55.6.893 |title=Racial ambivalence and American value conflict: Correlational and priming studies of dual cognitive structures |
Tara MacDonald and Mark Zanna suggested that [[
In a study testing the nature of ambivalent prejudice, Hisako Matsuo and Kevin McIntyre (2005)<ref name=Matsuo2005>{{cite conference |last1=Matsuo |first1=Hisako |last2=McIntyre |first2=Kevin P.|date=August 12, 2005 |title=Ambivalent Prejudice toward Immigrants: The Role of Social Contact and Ethnic Origin |conference=Annual meeting of the [[American Sociological Association]] |location=Marriott Hotel, [[Loews Philadelphia Hotel]], Philadelphia |url=http://citation.allacademic.com/meta/p_mla_apa_research_citation/0/2/0/1/1/pages20113/p20113-1.php}}</ref> studied American attitudes toward [[
==Measures==
Researchers use a variety of methods to measure ambivalent prejudice. The most widely used method is the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (ASI) for sexism created by Glick and Fiske in 1996.<ref name="Fiske2012"/><ref>G. Boyle & D. Saklofske (Eds.), ''Measures of Personality & Social Psychological Constructs''. Elsevier/Academic Press.</ref> Typical of all ingroup-
Women who resist traditional gender roles are punished by hostile sexism which resembles old-fashioned sexism. The theory predicts resentment of nontraditional women along each dimension: dominative paternalism, competitive gender differentiation, and heterosexual hostility.<ref name="Fiske2012"/> Conversely, women who co-operate with traditional gender roles and relationships evoke benevolent sexism, which comprises protective paternalism, complementary gender differentiation, and heterosexual intimacy. The ASI measures sexism along all of the six dimensions that compose hostile sexism and benevolent sexism.<ref name="Fiske2012"/>
The ASI is a self-report measure composed of 22 items, 11 for each subscale: hostile sexism and benevolent sexism. The two subscales can be calculated separately, or they can be averaged together to get an overall measure of sexism.<ref name="Fiske2012"/> The assessment consist of a series of statements with which respondents indicate their level of agreement on a 6-point Likert scale in which 0 = disagree strongly and 5 = agree strongly. Certain items are reversed coded so that agreement with the statement indicates lower levels of sexism and disagreement with the statement indicates higher levels of sexism. Example items from the ASI include:<ref name="Fiske2012"/>▼
▲The ASI is a self-report measure composed of 22 items, 11 for each subscale: hostile sexism and benevolent sexism.
Certain items are reversed coded so that agreement with the statement indicates lower levels of sexism and disagreement with the statement indicates higher levels of sexism. Example items from the ASI include:<ref name="Fiske2012"/>
Below is a series of statements concerning men and women and their relationships in contemporary society that this study wrote for their subjects to evaluate.<ref name="Fiske2012"/>
Line 46 ⟶ 51:
===Sexism===
[[Ambivalent sexism]] reflects the duality of hostility towards women and the tendency for women to be rated more positively than men in surveys. Hostile sexism impacts those perceived as nontraditional women who threaten male power, for example, female professionals and intellectuals, [[Feminism|feminists]], and [[Political lesbianism|political lesbians]]. Conversely, [[benevolent sexism]] protects women who
===Racism===
Ambivalent racism depicts two contrasting reactions by [[whites]] toward [[black people|blacks]]. These competing evaluations include hostile (antiblack) sentiments and subjectively sympathetic but paternalistic (problack) sentiments.<ref name="Fiske2012" /> Problack attitudes attribute black disadvantage to larger social structures and factors including [[discrimination]], [[Racial segregation|segregation]], and lack of opportunities. In contrast, hostile antiblack racism, like [[old-fashioned racism]] asserts that "black people are unambitious, disorganized, free-riding, and do not value education."<ref name="Fiske2012" /> Fiske states that "black [[Americans]] are viewed ambivalently mainly to the extent that white Americans simultaneously harbor a more subjectively positive and a more hostile attitude, which can flip from one polarity to the other, depending on individual differences in beliefs and on situational cues."<ref name="Fiske2012" />
===Ableism===
Söder suggests that people do not have fixed cognitive assumptions or emotions about people with [[Disability|disabilities]].<ref name=Soder1990>{{cite journal |last= Söder|first= Mårten|date= 1990|title= Prejudice or Ambivalence? Attitudes Toward Persons with Disabilities
===
Matsuo and McIntyre applied the concept of ambivalent prejudice to [[immigrants]] and [[refugees]].<ref name=Matsuo2005/> They described attitudes toward immigrants and refugees as ambivalent since on the one hand they are perceived "sympathetically, as disadvantaged, and deserving of justice", but on the other hand, they are seen as "more likely to be involved in crime and a burden on the public system."<ref name=Matsuo2005/> Matsuo and McIntyre used a sample survey of college students to test egalitarianism and the [[Protestant work ethic]] (PWE) and how it relates to perceptions of refugees.<ref name=Matsuo2005/> Participants completed survey questions regarding social contact, attitudes toward specific ethnic groups, general attitudes toward refugees, and the Humanitarianism/Protestant Work Ethic Scale. They found that the ambivalent attitudes toward refugees is based on the "dual maintenance of American values", egalitarianism and PWE. In testing the [[contact theory]], they found that only when contact is personal and cooperative does prejudice decrease.<ref name=Matsuo2005/>
==Response amplification==
In order to reduce the negative feelings brought about by cognitive dissonance, people may engage in response amplification.<ref name="KiteMary" /> Response amplification is defined by engaging in a more extreme response to a stigmatized individual in comparison to a similar but non-stigmatized individual than the situation calls for.<ref name="KiteMary" /> This can include overdoing both positive responses and negative responses depending on whether the situation calls for a positive or negative response.<ref name="KiteMary" />
For example, whites' evaluations of
In addition to racial contexts, response amplification has been found in multiple contexts including in cases of able bodied people interacting with disabled individuals, women and men rating members of the opposite sex, and ratings of female feminists.<ref name="KiteMary" />
==Mitigation==
Leippe and Eisenstadt found that dissonance mediated changed may be more successful when an internal conflict already exists, that is, when individuals possess cognitive dissonance that can be a result of ambivalence.<ref name=Leippe1994>{{cite journal |
Fiske suggests several methods to mitigate ambivalent prejudice particularly in the context of business management.<ref name="Fiske2012" /> These methods mainly involve an increased awareness and recognition of the different types of prejudice.<ref name="Fiske2012" /> She states that not all prejudices are alike, but they do create predictable groups of stereotypes, emotional prejudices, and discriminatory tendencies. When working to counteract prejudice, the focus should be on the most stereotypically negative aspect for a group, for example, competence for older people.<ref name="Fiske2012" /> In addition, constructive contact, that involving cooperation and equal status in the setting, for example, between groups improves [[emotional intelligence]].<ref name="Fiske2012" />
Line 73 ⟶ 78:
*[[Hostile prejudice]]
*[[Ingroups and outgroups]]
*[[Role
*[[Women are wonderful]]
*[[Aversive racism]]
*[[Tokenism]]
*[[Allosemitism]]
==References==
{{Reflist}}
[[Category:Prejudices]]
|