Jump to content

E-learning (theory)

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by BD2412 (talk | contribs) at 00:02, 7 September 2024 (Self-regulation: Clean up spacing around commas and other punctuation fixes, replaced: ,2004 → , 2004). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

E-learning theory describes the cognitive science principles of effective multimedia learning using electronic educational technology.

Multimedia instructional design principles

Beginning with cognitive load theory as their motivating scientific premise, researchers such as Richard E. Mayer, John Sweller, and Roxana Moreno established within the scientific literature a set of multimedia instructional design principles that promote effective learning.[1][2][3] Many of these principles have been "field tested" in everyday learning settings and found to be effective there as well.[4][5][6] The majority of this body of research has been performed using university students given relatively short lessons on technical concepts with which they held low prior knowledge.[7] However, David Roberts has tested the method with students in nine social science disciplines including sociology, politics and business studies. His longitudinal research program over 3 years established a clear improvement in levels of student engagement and in the development of active learning principles among students exposed to a combination of images and text over students exposed only to text.[8] A number of other studies have shown these principles to be effective with learners of other ages and with non-technical learning content.[9][10]

Research using learners who have greater prior knowledge of the lesson material sometimes finds results that contradict these design principles. This has led some researchers to put forward the "expertise effect" as an instructional design principle unto itself.[11][12][13][14]

The underlying theoretical premise, cognitive load theory, describes the amount of mental effort that is related to performing a task as falling into one of three categories: germane, intrinsic, and extraneous.[15]

  • Germane cognitive load: the mental effort required to process the task's information, make sense of it, and access and/or store it in long-term memory (for example, seeing a math problem, identifying the values and operations involved, and understanding that your task is to solve the math problem).
  • Intrinsic cognitive load: the mental effort required to perform the task itself (for example, actually solving the math problem).
  • Extraneous cognitive load: the mental effort imposed by the way that the task is delivered, which may or may not be efficient (for example, finding the math problem you are supposed to solve on a page that also contains advertisements for math books).

The multimedia instructional design principles identified by Mayer, Sweller, Moreno, and their colleagues are largely focused on minimizing extraneous cognitive load and managing intrinsic and germane loads at levels that are appropriate for the learner. Examples of these principles in practice include

  • Reducing extraneous load by eliminating visual and auditory effects and elements that are not central to the lesson, such as seductive details (the coherence principle)[16][17]
  • Reducing germane load by delivering verbal information through audio presentation (narration) while delivering relevant visual information through static images or animations (the modality principle)[18][19]
  • Controlling intrinsic load by breaking the lesson into smaller segments and giving learners control over the pace at which they move forward through the lesson material (the segmenting principle).[20][21][22]

Cognitive load theory (and by extension, many of the multimedia instructional design principles) is based in part on a model of working memory by Alan Baddeley and Graham Hitch, who proposed that working memory has two largely independent, limited capacity sub-components that tend to work in parallel – one visual and one verbal/acoustic.[23] This gave rise to dual-coding theory, first proposed by Allan Paivio and later applied to multimedia learning by Richard Mayer. According to Mayer,[24] separate channels of working memory process auditory and visual information during any lesson. Consequently, a learner can use more cognitive processing capacities to study materials that combine auditory verbal information with visual graphical information than to process materials that combine printed (visual) text with visual graphical information. In other words, the multi-modal materials reduce the cognitive load imposed on working memory.

In a series of studies, Mayer and his colleagues tested Paivio's dual-coding theory with multimedia lesson materials. They repeatedly found that students given multimedia with animation and narration consistently did better on transfer questions than those who learned from animation and text-based materials. That is, they were significantly better when it came to applying what they had learned after receiving multimedia rather than mono-media (visual only) instruction. These results were then later confirmed by other groups of researchers.

The initial studies of multimedia learning were limited to logical scientific processes that centered on cause-and-effect systems like automobile braking systems, how a bicycle pump works, or cloud formation. However, subsequent investigations found that the modality effect extended to other areas of learning.

Empirically established principles

  • Multimedia principle: Deeper learning is observed when words and relevant graphics are both presented than when words are presented alone (also called the multimedia effect).[25] Simply put, the three most common elements in multimedia presentations are relevant graphics, audio narration, and explanatory text. Combining any two of these three elements works better than using just one or all three.
  • Modality principle: Deeper learning occurs when graphics are explained by audio narration instead of on-screen text. Exceptions have been observed when learners are familiar with the content, are not native speakers of the narration language, or when only printed words appear on the screen.[25] Generally speaking, audio narration leads to better learning than the same words presented as text on the screen. This is especially true for walking someone through graphics on the screen and when the material to be learned is complex, or the terminology being used is already understood by the student (otherwise, see "pre-training"). One exception to this is when the learner will be using the information as a reference and will need to look back to it again and again.[26]
  • Coherence principle: Avoid including graphics, music, narration, and other content that does not support the learning. This helps focus the learner on the content they need to learn and minimizes cognitive load imposed on memory by irrelevant and possibly distracting content.[25] The less learners know about the lesson content, the easier it is for them to get distracted by anything shown that is not directly relevant to the lesson. For learners with greater prior knowledge, however, some motivational imagery may increase their interest and learning effectiveness.[27][28]
  • Contiguity principle: Keep related pieces of information together. Deeper learning occurs when relevant text (for example, a label) is placed close to graphics, when spoken words and graphics are presented at the same time, and when feedback is presented next to the answer given by the learner.[25]
  • Segmenting principle: Deeper learning occurs when content is broken into small chunks.[25] Break down long lessons into several shorter lessons. Break down long text passages into multiple shorter ones.
  • Signaling principle: The use of visual, auditory, or temporal cues to draw attention to critical elements of the lesson. Common techniques include arrows, circles, highlighting or bolding text, and pausing or vocal emphasis in narration.[25][29] Ending lesson segments after the critical information has been given may also serve as a signaling cue.[30]
  • Learner control principle: Deeper learning occurs when learners can control the rate at which they move forward through segmented content.[20][31][32] Learners tend to do best when the narration stops after a short, meaningful segment of content is given and the learner has to click a "continue" button in order to start the next segment. Some research suggests not overwhelming the learner with too many control options, however. Giving just pause and play buttons may work better than giving pause, play, fast forward, and reverse buttons.[32] Also, high prior-knowledge learners may learn better when the lesson moves forward automatically, but they have a pause button that allows them to stop when they choose to do so.[33][34][35]
  • Personalization principle: Deeper learning in multimedia lessons occur when learners experience a stronger social presence, as when a conversational script or learning agents are used.[25] The effect is best seen when the tone of voice is casual, informal, and in a 1st person ("I" or "we") or 2nd person ("you") voice.[36] For example, of the following two sentences, the second version conveys more of a casual, informal, conversational tone:
A. The learner should have the sense that someone is talking directly to them when they hear the narration.
B. Your learner should feel like someone is talking directly to them when they hear your narration.
Also, research suggests that using a polite tone of voice ("You may want to try multiplying both sides of the equation by 10.") leads to deeper learning for low prior knowledge learners than does a less polite, more directive tone of voice ("Multiply both sides of the equation by 10."), but may impair deeper learning in high prior knowledge learners.[37][38] Finally, adding pedagogical agents (computer characters) can help if used to reinforce important content. For example, have the character narrate the lesson, point out critical features in on-screen graphics, or visually demonstrate concepts to the learner.[39][40][41][42][43]
  • Pre-training principle: Deeper learning occurs when lessons present key concepts or vocabulary before presenting the processes or procedures related to those concepts.[25] According to Mayer, Mathias, and Wetzel,[44] "Before presenting a multimedia explanation, make sure learners visually recognize each major component, can name each component and can describe the major state changes of each component. In short, make sure learners build component models before presenting a cause-and-effect explanation of how a system works." However, others have noted that including pre-training content appears to be more important for low prior knowledge learners than for high prior knowledge learners.[45][46][47]
  • Redundancy principle: Deeper learning occurs when lesson graphics are explained by audio narration alone rather than audio narration and on-screen text.[25] This effect is stronger when the lesson is fast-paced, and the words are familiar to the learners. Exceptions to this principle include: screens with no visuals, learners who are not native speakers of the course language, and placement of only a few keywords on the screen (i.e., labeling critical elements of the graphic image).[48][49][50]
  • Expertise effect: Instructional methods, such as those described above, that are helpful to domain novices or low prior knowledge learners may have no effect or may even depress learning in high prior knowledge learners.[25][51][52][53]

Such principles may not apply outside of laboratory conditions. For example, Muller found that adding approximately 50% additional extraneous but interesting material did not result in any significant difference in learner performance.[54] There is ongoing debate concerning the mechanisms underlying these beneficial principles,[55] and on what boundary conditions may apply.[56]

Learning theories

Good pedagogical practice has a theory of learning at its core. However, no single best-practice e-learning standard has emerged. This may be unlikely given the range of learning and teaching styles, the potential ways technology can be implemented, and how educational technology itself is changing.[57] Various pedagogical approaches or learning theories may be considered in designing and interacting with e-learning programs.

Social-constructivist – this pedagogy is particularly well afforded by the use of discussion forums, blogs, wikis, and online collaborative activities. It is a collaborative approach that opens educational content creation to a wider group, including the students themselves. The One Laptop Per Child Foundation attempted to use a constructivist approach in its project.[58]

Laurillard's conversational model[59] is also particularly relevant to e-learning, and Gilly Salmon's Five-Stage Model is a pedagogical approach to the use of discussion boards.[60]

The cognitive perspective focuses on the cognitive processes involved in learning as well as how the brain works.[61]

The emotional perspective focuses on the emotional aspects of learning, like motivation, engagement, fun, etc.[62]

The behavioural perspective focuses on the skills and behavioural outcomes of the learning process. Role-playing and application to on-the-job settings.[63]

The contextual perspective focuses on the environmental and social aspects which can stimulate learning. Interaction with other people, collaborative discovery, and the importance of peer support as well as pressure.[64]

Mode neutral Convergence or promotion of 'transmodal' learning where online and classroom learners can coexist within one learning environment, thus encouraging interconnectivity and the harnessing of collective intelligence.[65]

For many theorists, it's the interaction between student and teacher and student and student in the online environment that enhances learning (Mayes and de Freitas 2004). Pask's theory that learning occurs through conversations about a subject which in turn helps to make knowledge explicit, has an obvious application to learning within a VLE.[66]

Salmon developed a five-stage model of e-learning and e-moderating that for some time has had a major influence where online courses and online discussion forums have been used.[67] In her five-stage model, individual access and the ability of students to use the technology are the first steps to involvement and achievement. The second step involves students creating an identity online and finding others with whom to interact; online socialization is a critical element of the e-learning process in this model. In step 3, students give and share information relevant to the course with each other. Collaborative interaction amongst students is central to step 4. The fifth step in Salmon's model involves students looking for benefits from the system and using resources from outside of it to deepen their learning. Throughout all of this, the tutor/teacher/lecturer fulfills the role of moderator or e-moderator, acting as a facilitator of student learning.

Some criticism is now beginning to emerge. Her model does not easily transfer to other contexts (she developed it with experience from an Open University distance learning course). It ignores the variety of learning approaches that are possible within computer-mediated communication (CMC) and the range of learning available theories (Moule 2007).

Self-regulation

Self-regulated learning refers to several concepts that play major roles in learning and which have significant relevance in e-learning.[68] explains that in order to develop self-regulation, learning courses should offer opportunities for students to practice strategies and skills by themselves. Self-regulation is also strongly related to a student's social sources, such as parents and teachers. Moreover, Steinberg (1996) found that high-achieving students usually have high-expectation parents who monitor their children closely.[69]

In the academic environment, self-regulated learners usually set their academic goals and monitor and react themselves in the process in order to achieve their goals. Schunk argues, "Students must regulate not only their actions but also their underlying achievement-related cognitions, beliefs, intentions and effects"(p. 359). Moreover, academic self-regulation also helps students develop confidence in their ability to perform well in e-learning courses.[69]

Theoretical framework

E-learning literature identifies an ecology of concepts from a bibliometric study were identified the most used concepts associated with the use of computers in learning contexts, e.g., computer-assisted instruction (CAI), computer-assisted learning (CAL), computer-based education (CBE), e-learning, learning management systems (LMS), self-directed learning (SDL), and massive open online courses (MOOC). All these concepts have two aspects in common: learning and computers, except the SDL concept, which derives from psychology and does not necessarily apply to computer usage. These concepts are yet to be studied in scientific research and stand in contrast to MOOCs. Nowadays, e-learning can also mean massive distribution of content and global classes for all Internet users. E-learning studies can be focused on three principal dimensions: users, technology, and services.[70]

Application of Learning theory (education) to E-Learning (theory)

As alluded to at the beginning of this section, the discussion of whether to use virtual or physical learning environments is unlikely to yield an answer in the current format. First, the efficacy of the learning environment may depend on the concept being taught.[71]  Additionally, comparisons provide differences in learning theories as explanations for the differences between virtual and physical environments as a post-mortem explanation.[72]  When virtual and physical environments were designed so that the same learning theories were employed by the students, (Physical Engagement, Cognitive Load, Embodied Encoding, Embodied Schemas, and Conceptual Salience), differences in post-test performance did not lie between physical vs. virtual, but instead in how the environment was designed to support the particular learning theory.[73]  

These findings suggest that as long as virtual learning environments are well designed[74] and able to emulate the most important aspects of the physical environment that they are intended to replicate or enhance, research that has been previously applied to physical models or environments can also be applied to virtual ones.[75][76] This means that it's possible to apply a wealth of research from physical learning theory to virtual environments. These virtual learning environments – once developed – can present cost-effective solutions to learning, concerning time invested in setting up, use, and iterative use.[77] Additionally, due to the relatively low cost, students are able to perform advanced analytical techniques without the cost of lab supplies.[78] Many even believe that when considering the appropriate affordances of each (virtual or physical) representation, a blend that uses both can further enhance student learning.[79]

Teacher use of technology

Computing technology was not created by teachers. There has been little consultation between those who promote its use in schools and those who teach with it. Decisions to purchase technology for education are very often political decisions. Most staff using these technologies did not grow up with them.[80] Training teachers to use computer technology did improve their confidence in its use, but there was considerable dissatisfaction with training content and style of delivery.[81] The communication element, in particular, was highlighted as the least satisfactory part of the training, by which many teachers meant the use of a VLE and discussion forums to deliver online training (Leask 2002). Technical support for online learning, lack of access to hardware, poor monitoring of teacher progress, and a lack of support by online tutors were just some of the issues raised by the asynchronous online delivery of training (Davies 2004).

Newer generation web 2.0 services provide customizable, inexpensive platforms for authoring and disseminating multimedia-rich e-learning courses and do not need specialized information technology (IT) support.[82]

Pedagogical theory may have application in encouraging and assessing online participation.[83] Assessment methods for online participation have been reviewed.[83]

See also

References

  1. ^ Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R., "Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning." Educational psychologist, 38(1), 43-52, 2003.
  2. ^ Moreno, R., & Mayer, R, "Interactive multimodal learning environments." Educational Psychology Review, 19(3), 309-326, 2007.
  3. ^ Clark, R. C., Nguyen, F., & Sweller, J., "Efficiency in learning: Evidence-based guidelines to manage cognitive load." John Wiley & Sons, 2011.
  4. ^ Harskamp, E. G., Mayer, R. E., & Suhre, C., "Does the modality principle for multimedia learning apply to science classrooms?" Learning and Instruction, 17(5), 465-477, 2007.
  5. ^ Chang, C. C., & Yang, F. Y., "Exploring the cognitive loads of high-school students as they learn concepts in web-based environments," Computers & Education, 55(2), 673-680, 2010.
  6. ^ Issa, N., Mayer, R. E., Schuller, M., Wang, E., Shapiro, M. B., & DaRosa, D. A., "Teaching for understanding in medical classrooms using multimedia design principles," Medical education, 47(4), 388-396, 2013.
  7. ^ TEDx Talks (2016-12-13), Visual feasts of the mind: matching how we teach to how we learn | David Roberts | TEDxLoughboroughU, retrieved 2017-01-05
  8. ^ "Life after Death by PowerPoint | Inspire – teaching and learning in the Social Sciences". inspiringsocsci.pressbooks.com. Retrieved 2017-01-05.
  9. ^ Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., & Sweller, J., "Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes," Journal of educational psychology, 87(2), 319, 1995.
  10. ^ Gerven, P. W., Paas, F., Merriënboer, J. J., Hendriks, M., & Schmidt, H. G., "The efficiency of multimedia learning into old age," British Journal of Educational Psychology, 73(4), 489-505, 2003.
  11. ^ Spanjers, I. A. E., Wouters, P., Van Gog, T., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., "An expertise reversal effect of segmentation in learning from animations," Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 46-52, 2011.
  12. ^ Spanjers, I. A., Wouters, P., Van Gog, T., & Van Merrienboer, J. J., "An expertise reversal effect of segmentation in learning from animated worked-out examples," Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 46-52, 2011.
  13. ^ Blayney, P., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. "Interactions between the isolated–interactive elements effect and levels of learner expertise: Experimental evidence from an accountancy class," Instructional Science, 38(3), 277-287, 2010.
  14. ^ Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J., "Incorporating learner experience into the design of multimedia instruction," Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 126–136, 2000.
  15. ^ Sweller, J (June 1988). "Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning". Cognitive Science 12 (2): 257–285.
  16. ^ Mayer, R.E., Bove, W., Bryman, A., Mars, R., & Tapangco, L., "When less is more: Meaningful learning from visual and verbal summaries of science textbook lessons." Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 64–73, 1996.
  17. ^ Harp, S.F., & Mayer, R.E. "How seductive details do their damage: A theory of cognitive interest in science learning." Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 414–434, 1998.
  18. ^ Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., & Sweller, J., "Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes." Journal of educational psychology, 87(2), 319, 1995.
  19. ^ Mayer, R.E., & Moreno, R., "A split-attention effect in multimedia learning: Evidence for dual coding hypothesis." Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 484–490, 1998.
  20. ^ a b Moreno, R., "Optimizing learning from animations by minimizing cognitive load: Cognitive and affective consequences of signaling and segmentation methods." Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 765–781, 2007.
  21. ^ Spanjers, I. A., van Gog, T., Wouters, P., & van Merriënboer, J. J., "Explaining the segmentation effect in learning from animations: The role of pausing and temporal cueing." Computers & Education, 59(2), 274-280, 2012.
  22. ^ Florax, M., & Ploetzner, R., "What contributes to the split-attention effect? Role of text segmentation, picture labeling, and spatial proximity." Learning and Instruction, 20, 216–224, 2010.
  23. ^ Baddeley, A.D.; G.J. Hitch (1974). "Working Memory". In Bower, G.A. (ed.). The psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory (PDF). Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press. pp. 47–89.[permanent dead link]
  24. ^ Mayer, R. E. (2001). Multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-78749-9.
  25. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Clark, Ruth C.; Mayer, Richard E. (2011). E-learning and the science of instruction: Proven guidelines for consumers and designers of multimedia learning (3rd ed.). John Wiley & Sons.
  26. ^ "Basic principles for online and multimedia learning". www.edgurus.com. EdGurus.com. Retrieved 13 March 2015.
  27. ^ Park, B., Moreno, R., Seufert, T., & Brü., "Does cognitive load moderate the seductive details effect? A multimedia study." Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 5-10, 2011.
  28. ^ Magner, U. I., Schwonke, R., Aleven, V., Popescu, O., & Renkl, A., "Triggering situational interest by decorative illustrations both fosters and hinders learning in computer-based learning environments." Learning and Instruction, 29, 141-152, 2014.
  29. ^ Crooks, S. M., Cheon, J., Inan, F., Ari, F., & Flores, R. "Modality and cueing in multimedia learning: Examining cognitive and perceptual explanations for the modality effect." Computers in Human Behavior, 28(3), 1063-1071, 2012.
  30. ^ Ibrahim, M., Antonenko, P. D., Greenwood, C. M., & Wheeler, D. "Effects of segmenting, signalling, and weeding on learning from educational video." Learning, Media and Technology, 37(3), 220-235, 2012.
  31. ^ Hasler, B. S., Kersten, B., & Sweller, J., "Learner control, cognitive load and instructional animation." Applied cognitive psychology, 21(6), 713-729, 2007.
  32. ^ a b Savoji, A.P.; Hassanabadi, H.; Fasihipour, Z. (2011). "The modality effect in learner-paced multimedia learning". Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. 30: 1488–1493. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.10.288.
  33. ^ Khacharem, A., Spanjers, I. A., Zoudji, B., Kalyuga, S., & Ripoll, H., "Using segmentation to support the learning from animated soccer scenes: An effect of prior knowledge." Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14(2), 154-160, 2013.
  34. ^ Spanjers, I. A., Wouters, P., Van Gog, T., & Van Merrienboer, J. J., "An expertise reversal effect of segmentation in learning from animated worked-out examples." Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 46-52, 2011.
  35. ^ Hatsidimitris, G., & Kalyuga, S., "Guided self-management of transient information in animations through pacing and sequencing strategies." Educational Technology Research and Development, 61(1), 91-105, 2013
  36. ^ Kartal, G., "Does language matter in multimedia learning? Personalization principle revisited." Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 615, 2010.
  37. ^ Wang, N., Johnson, W. L., Mayer, R. E., Rizzo, P., Shaw, E., & Collins, H., "The politeness effect: Pedagogical agents and learning outcomes." International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 66(2), 98-112, 2008.
  38. ^ McLaren, B.M., DeLeeuw, K.E., & Mayer, R.E. (2011). A politeness effect in learning with web-based intelligent tutors. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 69, 70–79, 2011.
  39. ^ Mayer, R.E., Dow, G., & Mayer, S., "Multimedia learning in an interactive self-explaining environment: What works in the design of agent-based microworlds?" Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 806–813, 2003.
  40. ^ Moreno, R., Mayer, R.E., Spires, H., & Lester, J., "The case for social agency in computer-based teaching: Do students learn more deeply when they interact with animated pedagogical agents?" Cognition and Instruction, 19, 177–214, 2001.
  41. ^ Atkinson, R.K. (2002). Optimizing learning from examples using animated pedagogical agents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 416–427, 2002.
  42. ^ Mayer, R. E., & DaPra, C. S., "An embodiment effect in computer-based learning with animated pedagogical agents." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18(3), 239, 2012.
  43. ^ Moreno, R., Reislein, M., & Ozogul, G., "Using virtual peers to guide visual attention during learning." Journal of Media Psychology: Theories, Methods, and Applications, 22(2), 52-60, 2010.
  44. ^ Mayer, R.E., Mathias, A., & Wetzell, K., "Fostering understanding of multimedia messages through pretraining: Evidence for a two-stage theory of mental model construction." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 147–154, 2002.
  45. ^ Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J., "Assimilating complex information." Learning and Instruction, 12, 61–86, 2002.
  46. ^ Ayres, P., "Impact of reducing intrinsic cognitive load on learning in a mathematical domain." Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(3), 287-298, 2006.
  47. ^ Clarke, T., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J., "The impact of sequencing and prior knowledge on learning mathematics through spreadsheet applications." Educational Technology Research and Development, 53(3), 15-24, 2005.
  48. ^ Moreno, R., & Mayer, R.E., "Verbal redundancy in multimedia learning: When reading helps listening." Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 156–163, 2002.
  49. ^ Scheiter, K., Schüler, A., Gerjets, P., Huk, T., & Hesse, F. W., "Extending multimedia research: How do prerequisite knowledge and reading comprehension affect learning from text and pictures." Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 73-84, 2014.
  50. ^ Chandler, P., & Sweller, J., "Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction." Cognition and Instruction, 8, 293-332, 1991.
  51. ^ Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. "Incorporating learner experience into the design of multimedia instruction." Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 126–136, 2000.
  52. ^ McLaren, B.M., DeLeeuw, K.E., & Mayer, R.E. "A politeness effect in learning with web-based intelligent tutors." International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 69, 70–79, 2011.
  53. ^ Magner, U. I., Schwonke, R., Aleven, V., Popescu, O., & Renkl, A. "Triggering situational interest by decorative illustrations both fosters and hinders learning in computer-based learning environments." Learning and Instruction, 29, 141-152, 2014.
  54. ^ Muller, D. A.; Lee, K. J.; Sharma, M. D. (2008). "Coherence or interest: Which is most important in online multimedia learning?". Australasian Journal of Educational Technology. 24 (2): 211–221. doi:10.14742/ajet.1223.
  55. ^ Tabbers, Martens, Merriënboer. "The modality effect in multimedia instructions" (PDF). Open University of the Netherlands. Retrieved 2012-01-25.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  56. ^ Reinwein (2012). "Does the Modality Effect Exist? and if So, Which Modality Effect?". Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 41 (1): 1–32. doi:10.1007/s10936-011-9180-4. PMID 21989625. S2CID 45875521.
  57. ^ Meredith, S. and B. Newton (2003). "Models of eLearning: Technology Promise vs Learner Needs Literature Review." The International Journal of Management Education 3(3).
  58. ^ Wiki.Laptop.org
  59. ^ "Informal description of Laurillard's Model". Archived from the original on 2012-08-07. Retrieved 2013-04-04.
  60. ^ E-moderating: The Key to Teaching and Learning Online – Gilly Salmon, Kogan Page, 2000, ISBN 0-7494-4085-6
  61. ^ Bloom, B. S., and D. R. Krathwohl. (1956). Taxonomy of Educational Objectives: Handbook 1
  62. ^ Bååth, J. A. (1982) "Distance Students' Learning – Empirical Findings and Theoretical Deliberations"
  63. ^ Areskog, N-H. (1995) The Tutorial Process – the Roles of Student Teacher and Tutor in a Long Term Perspective
  64. ^ Black, J. & McClintock, R. (1995) "An Interpretation Construction Approach to Constructivist Design."
  65. ^ Smith B, Reed P & Jones C (2008) 'Mode Neutral' pedagogy. European Journal of Open, Distance and E-learning."
  66. ^ Allen, I. E., J. Seaman, et al. (2007). Blending In: The Extent and Promise of Blended Education in the United States. Needham, M.A., The Sloan Consortium.
  67. ^ Salmon, G. (2005). E-moderating, the key to teaching and learning online. Routledge Falmer.
  68. ^ Zimmerman, B. J. (Eds.). (1998). "Self-regulated learning: From teaching to self-reflective practice". Guilford Publications. Retrieved 5 June 2020.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  69. ^ a b Peter E. Williams and Chan M. Hllman (Feb., 2004). Differences in self-regulation for online learning between first-and second-generation college students.Research in Higher Education, Vol. 45, No.1, pp. 71-82.https://www.jstor.org/stable/40197287
  70. ^ Aparicio, M.; Bacao, F.; Oliveira, T. (2016). "An e-Learning Theoretical Framework" (PDF). Educational Technology & Society. 19 (1): 292–307.
  71. ^ Chini, J. J., Madsen, A., Gire, E., Rebello, N. S., & Puntambekar, S. (2012). Exploration of factors that affect the comparative effectiveness of physical and virtual manipulatives in an undergraduate laboratory. Physical Review Special Topics - Physics Education Research, 8(1), 010113.
  72. ^ Barrett, T. J., Stull, A. T., Hsu, T. M., & Hegarty, M. (2015). Constrained interactivity for relating multiple representations in science: When virtual is better than real. Computers & Education, 81, 69–81. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2014.09.009
  73. ^ Rau, M. A., & Schmidt, T. A. (2019). Disentangling Conceptual and Embodied Mechanisms for Learning with Virtual and Physical Representations. In S. Isotani, E. Millán, A. Ogan, P. Hastings, B. McLaren, & R. Luckin (Eds.), Artificial Intelligence in Education (Vol. 11625, pp. 419–431). doi:10.1007/978-3-030-23204-7_35
  74. ^ Mayer, R. E.; R. Moreno (1998). "A Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning: Implications for Design Principles" (PDF).
  75. ^ Dawley, L., & Dede, C. (2014). Situated Learning in Virtual Worlds and Immersive Simulations. In J. M. Spector, M. D. Merrill, J. Elen, & M. J. Bishop (Eds.), Handbook of Research on Educational Communications and Technology (pp. 723–734). doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-3185-5_58
  76. ^ Yuan, Y., Lee, C.-Y., & Wang, C.-H. (2010). A comparison study of polyominoes explorations in a physical and virtual manipulative environment. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 26(4), 307–316. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2010.00352.x
  77. ^ Durmu, S., & Karakirik, E. (2006). VIRTUAL MANIPULATIVES IN MATHEMATICS EDUCATION: A THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK. The Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology, 5(1), 7.
  78. ^ de Jong, T., Linn, M. C., & Zacharia, Z. C. (2013). Physical and Virtual Laboratories in Science and Engineering Education. Science, 340(6130), 305–308. doi:10.1126/science.1230579
  79. ^ Olympiou, G., & Zacharia, Z. C. (2012). Blending physical and virtual manipulatives: An effort to improve students' conceptual understanding through science laboratory experimentation. Science Education, 96(1), 21–47. doi:10.1002/sce.20463
  80. ^ Laurillard, D. (2006). Rethinking University Teaching: a framework for the effective use of learning technologies. Abingdon, Oxon., RoutledgeFalmer.
  81. ^ Galanouli, D., C. Murphy, et al. (2004). "Teachers' perceptions of the effectiveness of ICT-competence training." Computers and Education 43(1-2): 63-79.
  82. ^ Tam CW, Eastwood A. Available, intuitive and free! Building e-learning modules using web 2.0 services.Med Teach. 2012;34(12):1078-80. doi:10.3109/0142159X.2012.731105
  83. ^ a b Ho, S. (2002). "Evaluating students' participation in on-line discussions". Archived from the original on 2016-05-21. Retrieved 2012-07-26.